Gallery of Wug-Shaped Curves

Bruce P. Hayes
Department of Linguistics
UCLA

A wug-shaped curve with color, beak, and feet


Download the paper

Short version (23 pages, to appear in Annual Review of Linguistics)
Long version (45 pages)


What is a wug-shaped curve and why is it of interest?

The wug-shaped curve depicts a frequency pattern widely found in quantitative studies of variable phenomena in linguistics. Indeed, it is so widespread that I believe its appearance may be meaningful from a theoretical viewpoint. Visually, the wug-shaped curve takes the form of two or more identical sigmoid (logistic) curves, spaced apart.

The wug-shaped curve is a natural consequence of probabilistic versions of Harmonic grammar, such as MaxEnt. Here is how the analysis is set up:  we divide the constraint set into two families, having different forms or teleologies:  Baseline constraints and Perturbers. We then plot the empirical data points, in the form of probabilities (zero to one) on the vertical axis, and Baseline probability on the horizontal axis. This is done  separately, in a different color, for the data series defined by violations of the Perturber constraints. We also plot the sigmoid lines themselves, which show the model fit -- ideally, the data points will cling to their respective sigmoids.

Here is the underlying research agenda:  along with some of my colleagues, I suspect that MaxEnt, or something like it, is correct for natural language, and that is why wug-shaped are ubiquitous in language data. You can judge for yourself by browsing through the images in this gallery, or by analyzing your own data in this way (see the last section for how).

Various people see various things in multiple sigmoids. It was Dustin Bowers who suggested to me that they look like wugs. The wug was invented and first drawn in 1958 by Jean Berko Gleason, in one of the most famous papers ever written in linguistics. In recent years, the wug has been adopted by the field of linguistics as a sort of mascot. The real wug is cuter than the mathematical one.

What is this web page for?

I've written an article about wug-shaped curves in linguistics, which you can download from the links at the top of this page, in either a long or short version. Even the long version doesn't have all of the cases I've compiled, and it seems that a web site would be the best format to display them all together, perhaps adding more in the future. Below, I've included all the cases I have studied, including the ones where the data don't look entirely pretty.

Browsing hints

For the scholarly reference sources behind all these curves, please follow the links or look at the bibliography section of my paper. In most cases, a link to a spreadsheet is included, which will tell you how I obtained the data, did the MaxEnt reanalysis, and plotted the curve. For a few cases, my spreadsheet is still messy and can't be shared yet, though you could ask.



Directory

Wug-shaped curves in phonology
Wug-shaped curves in phonetics
Wug-shaped curves in syntax
Wug-shaped curves in sociolinguistics
Wug-shaped curves in semantics/pragmatics
Wug-shaped curves in language change
Wug-shaped curves in sound symbolism

Some graphs used to diagnose theories

How I made the curves



Wug-shaped curves in phonology


Hungarian vowel harmony

Sources:  Hayes and Londe (2006), Hayes et al. (2009), Zuraw and Hayes (2017)
Y-axis:  how often a stem will take back suffixes in a wug-test experiment
Baseline constraints:  based on stem vowels that influence harmony; B is any back vowel, F any front rounded vowel.
Perturber constraints:  stem-final consonant environments (e.g., after sibilants) that favor front harmony
Spreadsheet (forthcoming), plotting script

Wug shaped curve in Hungarian vowel harmony

French liaison

Source:  Zuraw and Hayes (2017)
Y-axis:  likelihood of elision or liaison; for example, use of [l] instead of [la] for the feminine definite article
Baseline constraints:  lexical propensity of Word 2 to act as an h-aspiré word
Perturber constraints:  lexical propensity of Word 1 to appear in its isolation form
Spreadsheet, plotting script

Wug shaped curve in French liaison

Tagalog Nasal Substitution

Sources:  Zuraw (2000, 2010), Zuraw and Hayes (2017)
Y-axis:  how often a stem of a given type will undergo the process of Nasal Substitution
Baseline constraints:  related to place and manner of stem-initial consonant
Perturber constraints: propensity of a particular prefix to trigger the process
Spreadsheet, plotting script

Wug shaped curve for Tagalog Nasal Substitution

Inversion of Final Devoicing in Dutch

Source:  Ernestus and Baayen (2003)
Y-axis:  how often speakers guess that a stem-final obstruent (always voiceless when word-final) will appear as voiced when suffixed
Baseline constraints:  place and manner of stem final consonants, preceding consonant if any
Perturber constraints:  based on three degrees of vowel length in the stem
Spreadsheet (forthcoming), plotting script

Wug shaped curve for inverted Dutch Final Devoicing


Finnish genitive plurals

Sources:  Anttila (1997), Boersma and Hayes (2001), Goldwater and Johnson (2003)Hayes (in progress)
Y-axis:  how often a stem will take the longer [-den] allomorph of the genitive plural
Baseline constraint:  whether allomorph choice will result in two consecutive light syllables
Perturber constraints:  based on vowel height and weight of stem syllables. One perturber is inviolable (infinite weight) and therefore produces a flat line, not a sigmoid.
Spreadsheet, plotting script

Wug shaped curve in Finnish genitive plurals


Schwa/zero alternations in French:  Smith/Pater

Source:  Smith and Pater (2020)
Y-axis:  how often  zero shows up in French schwa/zero alternations
Baseline constraints:  whether schwa is inserted or deleted, consonants in environment
Perturber constraint:  whether deletion of a schwa creates clashing (adjacent) stressed syllables
Spreadsheet, plotting script

Deletion of schwa in French

Schwa/zero alternations in French:  Storme

Source:  Storme (2021)
Y-axis:  how often  zero shows up in French schwa/zero alternations
Baseline constraints:  the markedness of the cluster into which schwa is inserted (Hard, Medium, Easy), whether morphology is derivational or inflectional.
Perturber constraint:  a difference between Swiss and French native speakers, treated by Storme as stricter Dep(schwa) in the Swiss dialect. Thanks to Benjamin Storme for help with these data.

Spreadsheet, plotting script

Epenthesis of schwa in two French dialects

Stress placement in Hupa

Source:  Ryan (2019)
Y-axis:  probability of initial stress rather than second syllable stress
Baseline constraints:  weight of initial syllable
Perturber constraints:  weight of second syllable
Spreadsheet, plotting script

Data for stress placement in Hupa



Wug-shaped curves in phonetics

Perception of voicing based on closure duration and length of preceding vowel

Source:  Kluender et al. (1988)
Y-axis:  likelihood an experimental participant will perceive a voiced instead of a voiceless stop
Baseline constraint:  gradient, based on closure duration
Perturber constraint:  long vs. short preceding vowel
Spreadsheet, plotting script

Wug shaped curve in voicing perception

Perception of liquids based on F3 and phonotactic constraints

Source:  Massaro and Cohen (1983)
Y-axis:  likelihood that an experimental participant will perceive [r] as opposed to [l]
Baseline constraints:  F3 value of a synthesized liquid consonant
Perturber constraints:  violation of various phonotactic constraints, based on choice of preceding consonant (*[tl, *[sr, ?[vl, ??[vr)
Spreadsheet, plotting script

Wug shaped curve in r-l perception



Wug-shaped curves in syntax

Datives in English

Source:  Szmrecsanyi et al. (2017)
Y-axis:  how often the meaning of the dative construction will be expressed using NP NP rather than NP to NP
Baseline constraints:  governing various properties of the Recipient
Perturber constraints:  status of the Theme
Spreadsheet (forthcoming), plotting script

Wug shaped curve for English Dative constructions

Genitives in English

Source:  Szmrecsanyi et al. (2017)
Y-axis:  how often the meaning of the possessive will be expressed using NP's NP rather than NP of NP
Baseline constraints:  an amalgam; consult the Szmerecsanyi et al. paper
Perturber constraints:  based on length of possessor in words
Spreadsheet (forthcoming), plotting script

Wug shaped curve Szmrecsanyi et al Genitives

One can also plot the same data with length as the Perturber, like this:

Wug shaped curve in English genitives, length as perturber


Wug-shaped curves in sociolinguistics

Contraction of the copula in Black English

Sources:  Labov (1969), Cedergren and Sankoff (1974)
Y-axis:  how often the  speaker uses a contracted (vowelless) allomorph of the copula
Baseline constraints:  left side environment, including pronominal portmanteaux like he's
Perturber constraints:  right side syntactic environment
This case is unusual in my experience in that the data are fitted solely by the "tail" of the wug; cases further forward on the wug are empirically missing.
Spreadsheet, plotting script

Wug shaped curve for Labov Black English contraction

Deletion of the copula in Black English


Sources:  Labov (1969), Cedergren and Sankoff (1974)
Y-axis:  how often the  speaker uses a null allomorph of the copula, assuming they have already chosen to contract.
Baseline constraints:  left side environment, include pronominal portmanteaux like he's
Perturber constraints:  right side syntactic environment
Spreadsheet, plotting script
This is perhaps the messiest case I have seen; perhaps the use of conditional probability is the problem?

Black English deletion

Deletion of [l] in Quebec French

Source:  G. Sankoff (1972), cited and discussed in Bailey (1973)
Y-axis:  deletion rate of [l]
Baseline constraints:  varying propensity of various function words to lose their [l]
Perturber constraints:  sex and social class of speaker, taken as a proxy for Max(l) varying by speaking style
Spreadsheet, plotting script

Wug shaped curve for Quebec French [l] deletion

This case stands out as problematic for Stochastic OT, critiqued in  Zuraw and Hayes (2017) and my own paper. Here is a graph of a best-fit model of these data in Stochastic OT:

Sankoff/Bailey French data, ill modeled in Stochastic OT

Omission of que in Quebec French

Source:   Cedergren and Sankoff (1974)
Y-axis: retention rate for que 
Baseline constraints:  surrounding consonants
Perturber constraints:  formality of style, as varied by type of speaker
Spreadsheet, plotting script

Data for que deletion in Montreal French


R-Spirantization in Panamanian Spanish

Source: Cedergren and Sankoff (1974)
Y-axis: probability of realizing /r/ as a spirant
Baseline constraints:  phrasal position, whether /r/ is part of the infinitive ending, speaking style
Perturber constraints:  following segment
This is a rather messy one, I admit, and in particular lacks extreme values of probability.
Spreadsheet, plotting script

Data for r-spirantization in Panamanian Spanish

R-Dropping in New York City English

Source:  William Labov, via  Cedergren and Sankoff (1974)
Y-axis: probability of deleting /r/ in syllable codas
Baseline constraints:  speaking context
Perturber constraints:  designating different dialects spoken in the same speech community.
Spreadsheet, plotting script

Frequency of r dropping in New York City English

Cluster Simplification in Detroit Black English

Source:  Wolfram (1969)
Y-axis: probability of deleting one of a pair of adjacent consonants
Baseline constraints:  neighboring vowel/consonant, whether deleting consonant is part of past tense suffix
Perturber constraints:  social class, assumed to be a proxy for speaking style
This curve has a puzzling too-close vertical grouping for the _ C/ -ed case.
Spreadsheet, plotting script

Data for cluster simpllification in Detroit English


Wug-shaped curves in language change

Portuguese definite articles

Source:  Kroch (1989), ultimately from Oliveira y Silva (1982)
Y-axis: probability of use of a definite article when a NP also has an NP possessor
Baseline constraints:  rising constraint preferring this usage, over centuries
No perturber

Data for article appearance in Portuguese possessed noun phrases

Periphrastic do in  English

Source Kroch (1989), ultimately from Ellegard (1953).
Y-axis: probability of employing the inserted aux do.
Baseline constraints:  a preference constraint shifting over time
Perturber constraints:  governing various syntactic contexts.
Spreadsheet, plotting script

Data on use of do from Ellegard/Kroch


Evolution of have from Aux to main verb in  English

Source Zimmermann (2017) 
Y-axis: probability of employing have syntactically as a main verb rather than as an Aux
Baseline constraint:  a Aux-preferring constraint shifting over time
Perturber constraints:  governing various distinct uses of auxiliary verbs
Right graph plots same thing in different coordinates (harmony difference), showing identical slopes

Data on use of Have from Zimmermann 2017


Wug-shaped curves in semantics/pragmatics

Quantifier scope

Source:  AnderBois et al. (2012)
Y-axis: probability subjects will prefer narrow scope 
Baseline constraint:  whether the target quantifier is in first or second position
Perturber constraint: whether the target quantifier is in subject or object position
Spreadsheet, plotting script

Data from quantifier scope experiment


Wug-shaped curves in sound symbolism

Classification of Pokemon character names

Source (in this case, provides full analysis and discussion from a wug-shaped point of view):  Kawahara (2020)
Y-axis: probability subjects will rate a Pokemon name as appropriate for an "unevolved," smaller Pokemon creature 
Baseline constraints:  length of name in moras
Perturber constraints: whether name includes an initial voiced obstruent (such as [d])
Spreadsheet (forthcoming), plotting script

Data for Kawahara Pokemon study



Graphs used to diagnose theories

The  MaxEnt sigmoid

This is discussed extensively in the main text of my paper and is plotted here to permit the comparisons that follow.

The MaxEnt sigmoid

The asymmetrical sigmoid of classical Noisy Harmonic Grammar

In the classical version of Noisy Harmonic Grammar (Boersma and Pater 2016), the "noise" that makes the theory stochastic is added to the constraint weights, prior to Harmony computation. This ends up producing a sigmoid curve quite different from that of Maxent; it is asymmetrical, and the long tail can be shown to asymptote to a value above zero.

Asymmetrical sigmoid of constraint-noise NHG

The symmetrical, oddly-similar sigmoid of late-noise Noisy Harmonic Grammar

If, in designing a Noisy Harmonic Grammar framework, you add the "noise" to the completed Harmony values of candidates, you get a sigmoid that is remarkably similar to the MaxEnt sigmoid (even though the math is completely different). Here are the MaxEnt and late-noise Noisy Harmonic Grammar sigmoids superposed, with constraint weights suitably scaled to make the resemblance clear.

Resemblance of candidate-noise NHG to MaxEnt


Can a wug-shaped curve be fitted to any data?

Well, no; not under any sensible meaning of the word "fitted".  For instance, I opened up the spreadsheet for the Massaro-Cohen experiment reported above and replaced the empirical values from their experiment with random values. What happens is that the best-fit MaxEnt weights came out very low, the scattergram of model fit emerged as cloud-shaped, and the fitted sigmoids look like this:

Random violations for Massaro-Cohen

The slope of the sigmoid ever-so-vaguely fits an entirely random minor skewing of the data cloud, and the vertical arrangement of the sigmoids fits the  randomly imposed differences in the average values for  /r/ after different consonants.

So, no, good fit with wug-shaped curves doesn't come for free, it is a contingent fact about the data.  :=)

How I made the curves

I'm sure there are better ways (for instance, R is probably good) but this was the method I arrived at on an ad hoc basis. You can see examples of how all this works if you will download the spreadsheets and plotting scripts for individual cases above.

1. Obtain data.  Some authors web-post their data, other have the data printed in their article, and still others give just a graph. Even with the latter, it is not hard to use Microsoft Paint to get the values:  look at the bottom of the screen for vertical and horizontal coordinates of points in pixels.  Hover over the data points, and over the legend ticks, and put their values into a spreadsheet.  Then you can use arithmetic (or the handy Excel FORECAST() function) to convert pixels into real values.

2. If necessary, reduce data from individual tokens to types-plus-counts.  I do this by applying my little Typizer program to the rows of a spreadsheet, read in plain text form, containing just the constraint violations.

3. Do a MaxEnt analysis of the data, which is easily done in spreadsheet form.  The spreadsheets above show you do this; it helps also to know the basics of MaxEnt; for which you can read my paper. The key step requires you to deploy the Excel Solver (which is free, but must be activated), in order to calculate constraint weights.  During this stage, you should calculate Harmony in two columns, one for Baseline constraints and one for Perturber constraints, then use their sum to give the overall Harmony from which probabilities are calculated.

In doing the MaxEnt analysis, use this trick, assuming a particular input has two candidates A and B:  if Candidate B has one violation of Constraint X, record the violation in the spreadsheet not as a 1 in the Candidate B's row, but rather as a -1 in Candidate A's row. Then the B row ends up blank, other than the crucial frequency value for B.  The math will come out the same, and it gives you the harmony values in ways plottable as a single number, as described in the longer version of my paper.

4. Collate the data, keeping only Candidate A for each pair.  I perform this collation with formulas in the space below the main MaxEnt analysis. You must also collate the values for Observed Frequency, Base Harmony, and Perturber Harmony.  Optionally, you can include data for Counts, if you'd like to plot as small the datapoints that are not well-attested. It is also good to gather values for Predicted Frequency; then you can make a scattergram with Observed against Predicted, calculate correlation, and in general assess whether your MaxEnt model is a good model.

5. Within the spreadsheet, fill in the necessary fields to make a plot. These are shown in blue in the spreadsheets posted here, and also can be seen in the downloadable plotting scripts.

6. Clip the blue material out of your spreadsheet and save it as a text file, which is the plotting script.

7. Download my PlotSigmoids.exe program (Windows only, sorry!), put it in a new folder of your choice, click on it, drag a plotting script file onto the designated blank area of the interface. It will make a bmp image and put it into the "out" subfolder.  

Questions:   bhayes@humnet.ucla.edu.


Last updated July 2021